Computer vision may not be as good as thought

NewsGuard 100/100 Score

For years, scientists have been trying to teach computers how to see like humans, and recent research has seemed to show computers making progress in recognizing visual objects.

A new MIT study, however, cautions that this apparent success may be misleading because the tests being used are inadvertently stacked in favor of computers.

Computer vision is important for applications ranging from ‘intelligent' cars to visual prosthetics for the blind. Recent computational models show apparently impressive progress, boasting 60 percent success rates in classifying natural photographic image sets. These include the widely used Caltech101 database, intended to test computer vision algorithms against the variety of images seen in the real world.

However, James DiCarlo, , a neuroscientist in the McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT, graduate student Nicolas Pinto, and David Cox of The Rowland Harvard Institute argue that these image sets have design flaws that enable computers to succeed where they would fail with more authentically varied images. For example, photographers tend to center objects in a frame and to prefer certain views and contexts. The visual system, by contrast, encounters objects in a much broader range of conditions.

“The ease with which we recognize visual objects belies the computational difficulty of this feat,” explains DiCarlo, the senior author of the study in the online January 25 PLoS Computational Biology. “The core challenge is image variation. Any given object can cast innumerable images onto the retina depending on its position, distance, orientation, lighting, and background.”

The team exposed the flaws in current tests of computer object recognition by using a simple ‘toy' computer model inspired by the earliest steps in the brain's visual pathway. Artificial neurons with properties resembling those in the brain's primary visual cortex analyze each point in the image and capture low-level information about the position and orientation of line boundaries. The model lacks the more sophisticated analysis that happens in later stages of visual processing to extract information about higher-level features of the visual scene such as shapes, surfaces, or spaces between objects.

The researchers intended this model as a ‘straw man,' expecting it to fail as a way to establish a baseline. When they tested it on the Caltech101 images, however, the model did surprisingly well, with performance similar or better than five state-of-the-art object-recognition systems.

How could that be? “We suspected that the supposedly natural images in current computer vision tests do not really engage the central problem of variability, and that our intuitions about what makes objects hard or easy to recognize are incorrect,” Pinto explains.

To test this idea, the authors designed a more carefully controlled test. Using just two categories, planes and cars, they introduced variations in position, size, and orientation that better reflect the range of variation in the real world.

“With only two types of objects to distinguish, this test should have been easier for the ‘toy' computer model, but it proved harder,” Cox says. The team's conclusion: “Our model did well on the Caltech101 image set not because it is a good model but because the ‘natural' images fail to adequately capture real-world variability.”

As a result, the researchers argue for revamping the current standards and images used by the computer vision community to compare models and measure progress. Before computers can approach the performance of the human brain, they say, scientists must better understand why the task of object recognition is so difficult and the brain's abilities are so impressive.

This study was supported by the National Eye Institute, The Pew Charitable Trust, and The McKnight Foundation.

http://www.mit.edu/

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Obesity and high-fat diets linked to accelerated aging in brain blood vessels