Infants exposed to more speech than music, UW study reveals

Speech and music are the dominant elements of an infant's auditory environment. While past research has shown that speech plays a critical role in children's language development, less is known about the music that infants hear.

A new University of Washington study, published May 21 in Developmental Science, is the first to compare the amount of music and speech that children hear in infancy. Results showed that infants hear more spoken language than music, with the gap widening as the babies get older.

We wanted to get a snapshot of what's happening in infants' home environments. Quite a few studies have looked at how many words babies hear at home, and they've shown that it's the amount of infant-directed speech that's important in language development. We realized we don't know anything about what type of music babies are hearing and how it compares to speech."

Christina Zhao, corresponding author, UW research assistant professor of speech and hearing sciences

Researchers analyzed a dataset of daylong audio recordings collected in English-learning infants' home environments at ages 6, 10, 14, 18 and 24 months. At every age, infants were exposed to more music from an electronic device than an in-person source. This pattern was reversed for speech. While the percentage of speech intended for infants significantly increased with time, it stayed the same for music.

"We're shocked at how little music is in these recordings," said Zhao, who is also the director of the Lab for Early Auditory Perception (LEAP), housed in the Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences (I-LABS). "The majority of music is not intended for babies. We can imagine these are songs streaming in the background or on the radio in the car. A lot of it is just ambient."

This differs from the highly engaging, multi-sensory movement-oriented music intervention that Zhao and her team had previously implemented in lab settings. During these sessions, music played while infants were given instruments and researchers taught caregivers how to synchronize their babies' movement with music. A control group of babies then came to the lab just to play.

"We did that twice," Zhao said. "Both times, we saw the same result: that music intervention was enhancing infant's neural responses to speech sounds. That got us thinking about what would happen in the real world. This study is the first step into that bigger question."

Past studies have largely relied on qualitative and quantitative parental reports to examine musical input in infants' environments, but parents tend to overestimate the amount they talk or sing to their children.

This study closes the gap by analyzing daylong auditory recordings made with Language Environment Analysis (LENA) recording devices. The recordings, originally created for a separate study, documented infants' natural sound environment for up to 16 hours per day for two days at each recording age.

Researchers then crowdsourced the process of annotating the LENA data through the citizen science Zooniverse platform. Volunteers were asked to determine if there was speech or music in the clip. When speech or music was identified, listeners were then asked whether it came from an in-person or electronic source. Finally, they judged whether the speech or music was intended for a baby.

Since this research featured a limited sample, researchers are now interested in expanding their dataset to determine if the result can be generalized to different cultures and populations. A follow-up study will examine the same type of LENA recordings from infants in Latinx families. Since audio recordings lack context, researchers are also interested in when music moments are happening in infants' lives.

"We're curious to see whether music input is correlated with any developmental milestones later on for these babies," Zhao said. "We know speech input is highly correlated with later language skills. In our data, we see that speech and music input are not correlated -; so it's not like a family who tends to talk more will also have more music. We're trying to see if music contributes more independently to certain aspects of development."

Other co-authors were Lindsay Hippe, former UW undergraduate honors thesis student and incoming master's student in clinical research speech-language pathology; Victoria Hennessy, LEAP research assistant/lab manager; and Naja Ferjan Ramírez, assistant professor of linguistics and adjunct research professor for I-LABS. This study was funded by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders of the National Institutes of Health.

Source:
Journal reference:

Hippe, L., et al. (2024). Comparison of speech and music input in North American infants’ home environment over the first 2 years of life. Developmental Science. doi.org/10.1111/desc.13528.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Research shows brain synchronization between humans and dogs