Lateralization of the brain – the tendency for the left and right hemispheres to specialize in different functions – underlies the development of a left-to-right mental number line, according to a study in newborn chicks.
The study, published as a Reviewed Preprint in eLife and appearing today as the final version, is described by the editors as fundamental. They say the evidence presented is compelling and the results will be of interest to researchers studying numerical cognition, brain lateralization, and cognitive brain development more broadly.
Many people intuitively think of numbers as arranged along a mental line, either running from left to right, with smaller numbers on the left and larger on the right, or vice versa. This representation – often called the mental number line – is traditionally thought to develop through cultural experience, especially through reading and writing direction. However, research has shown evidence of a left-to-right mental number line in young infants and animals, challenging this assumption and suggesting that this spatial-numerical association may have biological roots.
Brain lateralization, also known as hemispheric specialization, refers to the idea that the two hemispheres of the brain are functionally different and have specialized roles in various cognitive processes.
Embryonic light exposure induces brain lateralization in domestic chicks, and enhances their spatial-numerical abilities and their tendency to 'count' from left to right. Although several models have proposed that the mental number line originates from brain lateralization, direct evidence has been lacking. Our study provides this evidence, showing that lateralization is essential for the emergence of left-to-right spatial-numerical associations."
Rosa Rugani, lead author, Professor, Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy
In domestic chicks, light exposure during embryonic development is known to promote brain lateralization. Therefore, Rugani and colleagues incubated 100 chick eggs, half exposed to light and half kept in the dark. This produced a group of strongly lateralised chicks, and a group of weakly lateralised chicks.
After hatching, the chicks were trained to locate food hidden behind the fourth bottle cap in a vertical row of 10 identical caps. Once they had learned this task, the team rotated the array of feeders by 90 degrees so that it was horizontal and tested whether the chicks would search for food in the fourth cap from the left or the right. Both caps constituted a plausible choice, allowing the researchers to observe any natural directional preferences.
Strongly lateralised chicks showed a clear preference for selecting the fourth cap from the left, suggesting a left-to-right number mapping. In contrast, weakly lateralised chicks showed no consistent directional preference, selecting at random between the two caps.
The researchers then repeated this test while covering either the chicks' left or right eye. Because each eye primarily sends signals to the opposite hemisphere of the brain, this allowed the team to determine which hemisphere was processing information during the experiment. When chicks used only their left eye (engaging the right hemisphere), those with strongly lateralised brains again chose the fourth cap from the left more frequently, reinforcing the role of the right hemisphere in integrating spatial and numerical cues. When these same chicks used only their right eye (engaging the left hemisphere) they tended to choose the fourth cap from the right instead. The weakly lateralised chicks failed to solve the task in both monocular conditions. This suggests that brain lateralization, established through light exposure during the last days of embryonic development, is essential for integrating spatial and numerical cues, and boosts cognitive performance.
In a follow-up experiment, the researchers tested how the chicks would perform if the spatial cues were made unreliable. They changed the spacing between the caps so that the physical position of the fourth cap varied – meaning the chicks could only rely on ordinal (numerical) information, not spatial layout, to locate the food. Under these conditions, neither strongly nor weakly lateralised chicks showed a directional preference.
"For the first time, we show that brain lateralization is not just related to the mental number line – it's necessary for it," says Rugani. "This finding brings strong experimental support to the idea that our sense of number and space is biologically grounded, yet shaped by an individual's interaction with the environment."
The authors suggest that a natural left-to-right scanning pattern may have evolutionary advantages for chicks. For example, when foraging it may allow them to efficiently locate and quantify food sources without overlooking areas.
"Our work demonstrates that lateralized brain function plays a key role in shaping how animals, possibly including humans, think about numbers," says senior author Lucia Regolin, a Professor in the Department of General Psychology, University of Padua. "Understanding the biological basis of numerical thinking may help us identify why certain cognitive abilities emerge when they do in development, and why they might be altered in individuals with atypical brain organisation. This research opens the door to further studies on the developmental origins of numerical reasoning, and how early sensory experiences can influence later cognitive outcomes."
Source:
Journal reference:
Rugani, R., et al. (2025). Hatching with Numbers: Pre-natal Light Exposure Affects Number Sense and the Mental Number Line in young domestic chicks. eLife. doi.org/10.7554/elife.106356