Inductive reasoning can play big role in how people perceive zoonosis risks, study reveals

NewsGuard 100/100 Score

These findings could help public health organizations craft more effective messages

If you were bitten by a bird, would you be concerned about getting sick? How likely would you be to seek medical attention? As it turns out, those answers may depend on your knowledge of other animals’ susceptibility to disease.

In psychology, it’s called inductive reasoning – that’s the process of generalizing information to novel scenarios. And according to a new study from the University of Sydney and Texas Tech University, inductive reasoning can play a big role in how people perceive the risks involved with animals and infectious diseases.

Dr Micah Goldwater from the University of Sydney’s School of Psychology collaborated with Texas Tech Assistant Professors Tyler Davis, Molly Irelandand Jason Van Allen and independent research consultant Nicholas Gaylord.

Their paper will appear in PLOS ONE, an open-access scientific journal published by the Public Library of Science.

Co-author at the University of Sydney, Dr Goldwater, explained that the World Health Organization and the Centre for Disease Control had different approaches to communicating about Ebola, with different results; while the CDC only listed bats and non-human primates, the WHO also listed porcupines and forest antelope.

“We showed that people reading the WHO inspired warning generalized further, and for example, judged that eating "bushmeat" (i.e., meat from wild animals) had greater risk,” Dr Golwater said. “This is a crucial result as eating bushmeat is a frequent root of new emerging diseases.”

The idea behind inductive reasoning is simple. Suppose you are bitten by an animal – maybe a bird, maybe a bat or maybe a dog. If you know that other similar animals are susceptible to communicable diseases, such as Ebola or rabies, you are likely to be more concerned about the possibility of getting infected by the bite. This happens because you generalise your knowledge from other animals to the one that bit you, even though that may not be entirely accurate.

“We’ve been interested for a while in how everyday people reason about risks associated with animal contact,” said Assistant Professor Davis, the lead author.

“An overwhelming number of new emerging diseases come from animal sources and get introduced to the human population as a result of animal contact.

“Thus, everyday people without expertise in infectious diseases or how to interact with animals are at the frontlines of potential future pandemics, yet very little is known about how they reason about the risks of animal contact.”

While a person may not know much about the risks posed by a specific animal, they likely have beliefs about which animals in general may be susceptible to disease.

Assistant Professor Davis highlighted that this study tested whether people use knowledge about the range of animals that are susceptible to a disease when judging their own risks of contact with a specific type of animal. The researchers measured this in a variety of ways, including the likelihood of reporting animal bites to a health professional and the perceived safety of eating different animals’ meat.

The study found that risk perception increases in two different scenarios. First, if the animal you encountered is similar to a type of animal you believe may carry a disease – for instance, encountering a coyote when you know that local foxes can carry a disease – you may perceive a greater risk to your own health. Second, if you know that a particular disease is found in a wide variety of animals, you may perceive a greater likelihood that the animal you encountered could carry it – for example, if bats, cats and birds all carry a disease, then the coyote you encountered may well pose a risk, as well.

“Although there has been a lot of research on inductive reasoning, this research has not been widely applied to health behaviors in general and perception of disease risk from animals in particular,” Assistant Professor Davis said.

“We’re also very hopeful that this work can inform better public health messaging in the developing world, where awareness of risks can be very low and responses to outbreaks are often slow and costly.”

Dr Goldwater said small changes to the message, informed by cognitive psychology, may have a big impact. “The next steps in our research are examining messages about food-borne illnesses in developed western countries, and bringing the research on bushmeat to the developing world,” he said.

Source: https://sydney.edu.au/

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Research explores the health benefits of resistant starch in plant-based diets